DOES SELF-COMPASSION MITIGATE THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN BURNOUT AND BARRIERS TO COMPASSION?
This longitudinal study examined how nightly variations in adolescents’ study and sleep time are associated with academic problems on the following day. Participants (N = 535, 9th grade Mage = 14.88) completed daily diaries every day for 14 days in 9th, 10th, and 12th grades. Results suggest that regardless of how much a stu- dent generally studies each day, if that student sacrifices sleep time to study more than usual, he or she will have more trouble understanding material taught in class and be more likely to struggle on an assignment or test the following day. Because students are increasingly likely to sacrifice sleep time for studying in the latter years of high school, this negative dynamic becomes increasingly prevalent over time.
Students generally learn best when they keep a con- sistent study schedule and distribute their study time evenly across a number of days (e.g., Bahrick & Phelps, 1987; Dempster & Farris, 1990). Although this paced learning is ideal, the increasing demands that high school students face may make such a consistent schedule infeasible. Socializing with peers and working for pay, for example, both increase across the course of high school (Shanahan & Flaherty, 2001; Wight, Price, Bianchi, & Hunt, 2009). As they advance through high school, adoles- cents’ academic obligations also intensify and often require more time and effort (Eccles et al., 1993). As a result, many high school students end up with irregular study schedules, often facing nights in which they need to spend substantially more time than usual studying or completing school work.
Although these nights of extra studying may seem necessary, they can come at a cost. When allo- cating their limited number of hours across a num- ber of activities, adolescents often make trade-offs, sacrificing certain activities to make time for others.
Among adolescents, sleep is one activity that is commonly sacrificed for additional study time (Adam, Snell, & Pendry, 2007). Indeed, study time is one of the most significant determinants of high school students’ sleep time, more so than time spent with friends or family or time spent using media (e.g., computers or television; Fuligni & Hardway, 2006). Even adolescents themselves report that too much homework is a common barrier that prevents them from getting enough sleep (Noland, Price, Dake, & Telljohann, 2009). Studying is obviously a contributor to academic achievement, yet adequate sleep is also important for academics (Curcio, Ferr- ara, & De Gennaro, 2006). Thus, it is possible that nights of extra studying are not as effective as stu- dents think. In fact, these nights of extra studying may even be counterproductive because they are likely to also be nights of less sleep and sleep sche- dule irregularity, both of which can interfere with school performance (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998).
In high school, sacrificing sleep to study may be especially problematic because, in general, high school age adolescents are chronically sleep deprived (Carskadon, 1990). Although the optimal amount of sleep varies somewhat across individu- als, most adolescents need just over 9 hr of sleep each night (Wolfson & Carskadon, 1998). Only
Support for this study was provided by a grant from the Rus- sell Sage Foundation. We are extremely grateful to the principals and teachers who welcomed us into their schools and to the stu- dents who shared their daily lives with us.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Cari Gillen-O’Neel, Department of Psychology, University of California, 1285 Franz Hall, Los Angeles, CA 90095-1563. Elec- tronic mail may be sent to c.go@ucla.edu.
[Correction added on 9/7/2012, after first online publication 8/20/2012: Virginia W. Huynh’s affiliation has been corrected to California State University, Northridge.]
Child Development, January/February 2013, Volume 84, Number 1, Pages 133–142
� 2012 The Authors Child Development � 2012 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2013/8401-0011
DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01834.x
about 9% of high school students, however, sleep for at least the requisite 9 hr per night (National Sleep Foundation, 2006). One fourth of high school students get a borderline amount of sleep (between 8 and 9 hr per night), and the vast majority of high school students (62%) get insufficient sleep (< 8 hr per night; National Sleep Foundation, 2006).
Across the course of high school, the biologically needed hours of sleep remain constant, yet the average amount that students sleep declines (Cars- kadon, Acebo, & Jenni, 2004). In 9th grade, the average adolescent sleeps for 7.6 hr per night, and this time decreases to 7.3 hr in 10th grade, 7.0 hr in 11th grade, and 6.9 hr in 12th grade (National Sleep Foundation, 2006). Thus, adolescents start high school sleeping for fewer hours than they need, and this sleep deprivation worsens over the course of high school (Fukuda & Ishihara, 2001).
Presumably, when students choose to trade sleep for studying, they do so because they believe that the increased studying will help their grades. On the one hand, this strategy may be effective because, overall, study time is associated with aca- demic achievement. On average, students who spend more time studying tend to do better on achievement tests (Fuligni & Stevenson, 1995). Stu- dents who have high grade point averages (GPAs) study for an average of about three fourths of an hour longer on weeknights than their peers who have low GPAs (Witkow, 2009). On the other hand, sacrificing sleep, even for additional studying, may be an ineffective strategy because average time spent sleeping also contributes to higher achieve- ment. Students whose schedules include more hours of sleep per night tend to have higher grades than their peers who sleep for fewer hours (Wolf- son & Carskadon, 2003). When students are sleep deprived, they experience greater fatigue at school the following day, and greater fatigue can make learning more difficult (Giannotti, Cortesi, Sebas- tiani, & Ottaviano, 2002). Furthermore, sleep is a key restorative process during which consolidation of learning takes place (Diekelmann & Born, 2010). Thus, additional study time, when it comes at the expense of sleep time, may not benefit achievement as much as students think.
Current Study
Most research examining studying, sleep, and achievement have focused on average study times, average sleep times, and broad indicators of aca- demic achievement such as GPAs and test scores (e.g., Taras & Potts-Datema, 2005; Wolfson & Cars-
kadon, 2003). Yet, average study and sleep times are the aggregated result of the daily demands on adolescents’ time and the daily time-allocation choices that they make. Likewise, GPAs and test scores are the result of daily behaviors such as pay- ing attention in class and doing well on assign- ments and examinations. Despite the fact that studying, sleep, and school are all daily occur- rences, most previous examinations of these issues have only examined associations at the aggregate level, leaving the daily dynamics of studying, sleep, and academics unexplored.
In this longitudinal study, we examined how daily choices regarding study time and sleep time are associated with adolescents’ academic behaviors at school on the following day, and how these daily dynamics changed over the course of high school. For 14 days in each the 9th, 10th, and 12th grades, we obtained daily reports of study time, sleep time, and academic functioning from an ethnically diverse sample of students. Because our data were collected among the same students over the course of many days, we could conduct within-person analyses that essentially allow participants to serve as their own control group and, therefore, control for unmea- sured factors that can confound traditional, between-person analyses. In other words, we could move beyond between-person questions (e.g., do the students with the longest average study times tend to be the students with more or fewer academic problems?) and instead answer within-person, daily-level questions (e.g., when a student studies longer than usual on a particular day, what happens to this student’s academic problems on the following day?). Specifically, we sought to answer the follow- ing questions: (a) Do adolescents have better or worse academic performance on days after they spent more time than usual studying and doing homework? (b) Does extra time spent studying cut into adolescents’ sleep on a daily basis? and (c) Does reduced sleep either diminish any positive effects of extra studying or explain any possible negative effects of extra studying on academic performance? Furthermore, by following the same participants over multiple years of high school, we were also able to examine how the dynamics of study time, sleep time, and academic functioning change over time.
Method
Participants and Procedure
Beginning in 9th and continuing in 10th and 12th grades, we recruited students from three Los
134 Gillen-O’Neel, Huynh, and Fuligni
Angeles public high schools. The first school pri- marily served students from Latin American and Asian backgrounds whose families had lower-mid- dle- to middle-class educational and occupational statuses, the second school primarily served lower- middle- to middle-class students from Latin Ameri- can and European backgrounds, and the third school primarily served middle- to upper-middle- class students from Asian and European back- grounds (California Department of Education, 2006). In the first two schools, we invited all stu- dents in the target grade to participate in each year of the study. Due to the large size of the third school, we invited approximately half the 9th grad- ers to participate the first year of the study, and we only followed these students in 10th and 12th grades. At all three schools, students who had par- ticipated in earlier years but were no longer enrolled in the school were contacted in subsequent years and invited to participate by mail.
Participants were recruited during school hours in the spring semesters of each school year. Stu- dents who returned parental consent forms and provided their own assent completed an initial questionnaire, also during school hours. This ques- tionnaire assessed students’ demographic informa- tion such as their gender, ethnicity, and the countries of birth for themselves and both parents. After completing the questionnaire, students received a 14-day supply of diary checklists and were instructed to fully complete one checklist each night before going to bed over the subsequent 2- week period. Each checklist was three pages long and took about 5–10 min to complete. To monitor on-time completion of the checklists, we provided participants with 14 manila envelopes and an elec- tronic time stamper (a hand-held device that imprints the current date and time and is pro- grammed with a security code to prevent alterations to the correct date). Participants were instructed to seal their completed checklists into an envelope each night and to stamp the seal with the time stam- per. At the end of the 2-week period, participants returned the completed materials to their school and received $30 compensation. If inspection of the data indicated that they had completed the check- lists correctly and on time, participants also received two movie passes. The time-stamper monitoring and the cash and movie pass incentives resulted in a high rate of compliance: Depending on the year of data collection, participants in our sample com- pleted 97%–99% of the possible 14 diaries (9th grade: M = 13.79, SD = 1.25; 10th grade: M = 13.81, SD = 1.13; 12th grade M = 13.65, SD = 1.52).
For this study, we examined responses from the 535 (52.1% female) Latino (n = 193), Asian American (n = 233), and European American students (n = 109) who reported on at least 7 of 14 days in at least 2 of the 3 years of data collection. Most of the Latino partici- pants (82.4%) were from Mexican backgrounds, and most of the Asian American participants (67.8%) were from Chinese backgrounds. During the first wave of data collection, participants ranged in age from 13.94 to 16.22 (M = 14.88, SD = 0.39).
As a measure of socioeconomic status (SES), we combined students’ reports of their parents’ educa- tional attainment and occupations. Students reported how far their mothers and fathers went in school by selecting one of the following categories: elementary or junior high school, some high school, graduated from high school, some college, graduated from college, or law, medical, or graduate school. Students’ open-ended reports of their mothers’ and fathers’ jobs were coded into the following five categories: unskilled (e.g., food service worker, parking attendant), semiskilled (e.g., construction worker, bus driver), skilled (e.g., nursing assistant, mechanic), semiprofessional (e.g., accoun- tant, social worker), or professional (e.g., attorney, dentist). The measures of parental education and occupation were each standardized and averaged to provide an overall index of SES. On average, stu- dents from European backgrounds reported higher SES (M = 0.633, SD = 0.540) than students from Asian backgrounds (M = )0.002, SD = 0.788), who in turn, reported higher SES than students from Latin American backgrounds (M = )0.586, SD = 0.700), F(2, 529) = 104.79, p < .001, g2 = .28.
Measures
Daily study and sleep time. Each evening for 14 days, participants reported how much time (in hours and minutes) they spent studying outside of school and how much time they slept the previous night. For study time, participants first checked (yes or no) whether or not they studied or did home- work while not in school and then gave a free response to the question: ‘‘(If yes) for how long?’’ For sleep time, participants answered the question: ‘‘How many hours and minutes did you sleep last night (for example, 7½ hr)?’’ Each year, the 14 response days spanned 10 school nights (i.e., Sun- day through Thursday) and 4 weekend nights. Given that our outcome of interest occurred on school days, we only used school-night reports of study and sleep time.
In comparison to surveys that often ask partici- pants to retrospectively report behaviors from the
To Study or to Sleep? 135
previous week, month, or year, daily reports depend less on memory and therefore are less susceptible to errors of estimation. For sleep time in particular, daily reports such as ours are highly correlated with more objective measures of sleep duration, such as those derived from wrist actigraphs (i.e., watch-like devices that measure sleep by analyzing body move- ments) and polysomnographic recordings (Lockley, Skene, & Arendt, 1999; Wolfson et al., 2003). Furthermore, daily reports of sleep time are mean- ingfully associated with other daily experiences and mood (e.g., fatigue; Fuligni & Hardway, 2006). For study time, the validity of our participants’ reports was indicated by the association between study time (averaged across all days and years) and students’ GPAs across years (r = .50, p < .001).
Daily academic problems. On each school day, participants indicated (yes or no) whether or not they had various experiences at school. As a mea- sure of daily academic problems, we summed par- ticipants’ responses to two items: ‘‘did not understand something taught in class’’ and ‘‘did poorly on a test, quiz, or homework’’ (range = 0–2). As a control variable, we also used participants’ daily responses to whether or not they ‘‘had a test or a quiz at school.’’ The validity of our measure of academic problems was indicated by the negative association between students’ reports of academic problems (averaged across all days and years) and students’ GPAs across years (r = ).12, p = .005).
Analytical Plan
First, we averaged daily reports of study times, hours of sleep, and academic problems from each grade and used a series of repeated measures anal- yses of variance to examine how students’ average daily study times, average hours of school-night sleep, and average number of academic problems changed across the course of high school. Then, we estimated a series of three-level hierarchical linear models (HLMs; Bryk & Raudenbush, 1992) to
examine daily-level associations between study time, sleep time, and academic problems and whether these associations changed over the course of high school.
Results
Study Time, Sleep Time, and Academic Problems Across the Course of High School
As shown in Table 1, study time did not change across the years of high school; in 9th, 10th, and 12th grades, students spent an average of just over an hour studying each school night. Sleep time, however, decreased over the course of high school. By the 12th grade, students slept for an average of 41.4 fewer minutes each school night than they did in 9th grade. Finally, the frequency of academic problems also decreased. In the 9th and 10th grades, students reported an average of one aca- demic problem every 3 days; by the 12th grade, the frequency of academic problems decreased to one problem every 5 days.
Academic Problems After Days With Extra Study Time
To examine whether adolescents had more or fewer academic problems on days after they spent more time studying than usual, we estimated an HLM using the following equations:
academic problemstþ1ij
¼ p0ij þ p1ijðprior-day study timetijÞ þ p2ijðhad a testtþ1ijÞþ etij
ð1Þ
p0ij ¼ b00j þ b01jðyearijÞþ r0ij ð2Þ
p1ij ¼ b10j þ b11jðyearijÞ ð3Þ
p2ij ¼ b20j þ b21jðyearijÞ ð4Þ
Table 1
Average Daily Study Time, Sleep Time, and Academic Problems Across the Course of High School
9th Grade 10th Grade 12th Grade
Statistical testM (SD) M (SD) M (SD)
Study time 1.12 (0.85) 1.10 (0.90) 1.06 (1.00) F(2, 732) = 0.32, p = .72, g2 = .001 Sleep time 7.63 (0.93) 7.40 (0.92) 6.94 (0.95) F(2, 730) = 112.45, p < .001, g2 = .236 Academic problems 0.33 (0.30) 0.32 (0.28) 0.20 (0.21) F(2, 732) = 27.88, p < .001, g2 = .071
Note. Both study time and sleep time are reported in hours. Academic problems could range from 0 to 2 each day.
136 Gillen-O’Neel, Huynh, and Fuligni
Study time was centered within each adolescent within each year. The Level 1 (daily-level) equation is shown as Equation 1: Adolescents’ academic problems on a given day are modeled as a function of their average daily academic problems (p0ij), the extent to which the time they spent studying on the prior night deviated from their personal norm (p1ij), and whether or not they had a test at school that day (p2ij). Equations 2–4 represent Level 2 (yearly level) effects, allowing us to examine the extent to which daily academic problems and the association between prior-day study time and academic prob- lems change over the years of high school. Years were coded as 0 = 9th grade, 1 = 10th grade, and 3 = 12th grade. This analysis also accounted for the nesting of observations within participants (Level 3), but these equations are not shown here because the model did not include any Level 3 predictors.
As shown in the first column and third row of Table 2, 9th-grade students’ deviation from their average study time had no association with their academic problems on the following day. Across the course of high school, however, the association between study time and academic problems chan- ged such that study time became increasingly asso- ciated with academic problems (see the first
column and fourth row of Table 2). As shown in Figure 1, reestimating our HLM—first with year centered at 10th grade and then with year centered at 12th grade—indicated that in 10th grade, days on which students reported longer than normal study times tended to be followed by days with more academic problems, and this daily association was even stronger in 12th grade.
It is important to note that, in Equation 1, we controlled for students’ daily reports of whether or not they had a test or quiz at school. Including this control rules out the possibility that nights of increased studying were followed by days with more academic problems only because students were both more likely to study and to have aca- demic problems when they have tests. Thus, our results suggest that regardless of whether or not students had a test, study time became increasingly associated with academic problems such that, by 10th grade, nights with longer than average study times tended to be followed by days with more aca- demic problems.
The Level 3 (individual-level) variance compo- nents for all effects of interest were nonsignificant. In other words, neither the 9th-grade association, nor the yearly change in the association between study time and academic problems differed across individuals. As such, we did not examine individ- ual-level differences (e.g., gender, ethnic, or SES differences) in these associations.
Table 2
Association Between Daily Study Time and Next Day’s Academic
Problems, Before and After Controlling for Daily Sleep Time
Academic problems